
Environmental psychology must better integrate local cultural and sociodemographic context to inform conservation
Author(s) -
Parathian Hannah E,
FrazãoMoreira Amélia,
Hockings Kimberley J
Publication year - 2018
Publication title -
conservation letters
Language(s) - English
Resource type - Journals
SCImago Journal Rank - 3.153
H-Index - 79
ISSN - 1755-263X
DOI - 10.1111/conl.12590
Subject(s) - wildlife , stewardship (theology) , context (archaeology) , wildlife conservation , ethnic group , environmental stewardship , cultural diversity , environmental ethics , sociology , cultural psychology , social psychology , geography , environmental resource management , psychology , political science , ecology , politics , anthropology , philosophy , environmental science , archaeology , law , biology
Finding solutions to urgent and complex conservation problems requires innovative research that draws on various disciplines. St. John et al. (2018) argue that models from psychology help elucidate how people make judgments about wildlife, and hence provide a novel framework for informing conservation. While this landscape-wide approach identifies some of the cognitive factors important for understanding human–wildlife coexistence, we believe the study inadequately incorporates the influence of local cultures, customs and habits on how people interact with wildlife. We raise two main points of contention: (i) the insufficient reference to local culture and exclusion of social diversity from the analysis and (ii) the inaccuracy of the claim that “sociodemographic characteristics generally fail to reveal underlying differences in how people relate to wildlife.” The authors strongly allude to the concurrence of animist and Islamic ontologies among the Sumatran peoples in their study. Despite this, the values and principles of neither are explained or integrated into analyses. The authors mention three ethnic groups (Minangkabau, Melayu, Kerincinese) without describing the main differences between these groups. This creates a gross over simplification of the local cultural context of human–wildlife relationships which could be problematic, particularly given results from other studies, which demonstrate that incorporating intercultural views into conservation strategy supports flexible policies that are culturally respectful (e.g., Moorcroft et al., 2012). The categories of analysis chosen by the authors are grounded in western rather than local concepts. For example, in Figure 1, “intolerance” and “stewardship” are shown as a continuum based upon people's behaviours which assumes that killing (an animal) represents intolerance. An abundance of ethnographies suggest that hunters do not always kill wildlife for this reason, and hunters can respect, be intrigued by, and possess in-depth knowledge about the animals they hunt (Kohn, 2013). Such concepts underlie hunter-prey relationships cross-culturally, and yet these ideas are overlooked by the semantic scales used in the study (good-bad; harmlessdangerous). Moreover, these categories are not fully defined by the authors which might prompt inaccurate conclusions.